Aproximación cognitiva a lo fantástico como vínculo: la teoría de las apercepciones. Definición y aplicaciones en relatos de Julio Cortázar, Jorge Luis Borges y Ángel Olgoso

Autores/as

  • Paolo Remorini Universidad de Granada

Resumen

Con este trabajo desarrollamos y damos a conocer una nueva teoría de lo fantástico denominada teoría de las apercepciones. Teniendo en cuenta las investigaciones de las ciencias cognitivas de las últimas décadas, sobre todo las relacionadas con la simulación encarnada, las neuronas espejo y el espacio de intersubjetividad, definimos lo fantástico como el vínculo que puede surgir de un desfase de las apercepciones como anomalía, alteración o transgresión de al menos uno de los niveles cognitivos y hermenéuticos que operan en cada experiencia narrativa.

El enfoque cognitivo permite asimismo superar las críticas sobre lo fantástico como posible género literario autónomo o como modalidad narrativa que se implanta sobre distintos tipos de narración, mientras que cobra importancia el grado de profundidad del vínculo fantástico entre las apercepciones del lector y el texto. A tal fin, presentamos un posible esquema analítico de investigación de textos narrativos.

Palabras clave

fantástico, ciencias cognitivas, apercepciones, simulación encarnada, intersubjetividad

Citas

ALTAVILLA, Daniela, Ines ADORNETTI, Alessandra CHIERA, Valentina DERIU, Alessandro ACCIAI y Francesco FERRETTI (2022): Introspective self-narrative modulates the neuronal response during the emphatic process: an event-related potentials (ERPs) study, Experimental Brain Research 240, pp. 2725-2738. <https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-022-06441-4>.

BARRENECHEA, Ana María (1972): Ensayo de una tipología de la literatura fantástica, Revista Iberoamericana, 80, pp. 391-403. <https://doi.org/10.5195/reviberoamer.1972.2727>.

BESSIÈRE, Irène (1973): Le récit fantastique. la poétique de l'incertain. Larousse, París.

BONINI, Luca, Cristina ROTUNNO, Edoardo ARCURI y Vittorio GALLESE (2022): Mirror neurons 30 years later: implications and applications, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26 (9), pp. 767-781. <https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.003>.

BORGES, Jorge Luis (1944): Ficciones, Emecé, Buenos Aires.

CAILLOIS, Roger (1965): Au coeur du fantastique, Gallimard, París.

CARACCIOLO, Marco, Cécile GUÉDON, Karin KUKKONEN y Sabine MÜLLER (2017): The Promise of an Embodied Narratology. Integrating Cognition Representation and Interpretation, in Per Krogh Hansen, John Pier, Philippe Roussin y Wolf Schmid (eds.), Emerging Vectors of Narratology, De Gruyter, Berlin, Boston, pp. 435-460. <https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110555158-021>.

CARNEVALE, Davide (2019): La parola fantástica: Logopoiesi, retoriche dell'indicibile e mostri verbali, in Benedetta Aldinucci, Valentina Carbonara, Giuseppe Caruso, Matteo La Grassa, Cèlia Nadal Pasqual y Eugenio Salvatore (eds.), Parola. una nozione unica per una ricerca multidisciplinare, Università per Stranieri di Siena, Siena, pp. 65-73.

CELY ÁVILA, Flor Emilce (2014): Intersubjetividad: entre explicación y comprensión, Revista colombiana de Psiquiatría, 43 (1), pp. 52-57. <https://doi.org/10.1016/S0034-7450(14)70043-5>.

CESERANI, Remo (1996): Il fantastico, Il Mulino, Bologna.

CLARK, Andy (2013): Whatever next? predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, pp. 181-204. <https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x12000477>.

COPLAN, Amy (2004): Empathic Engagement with Narrative Fictions, The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 62 (2), pp. 141-152. <https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-594X.2004.00147.x>.

CORTÁZAR, Julio (1969): Último round, Siglo XXI Editores, México.

CORTÁZAR, Julio (1963): Rayuela, Sudamericana, Buenos Aires.

DAMASIO, Antonio, y Gil B. CARVALHO (2013): The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, pp. 143-152. <https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3403>.

DI PELLEGRINO, Giuseppe, Luciano FADIGA, Leonardo FOGASSI, Vittorio GALLESE y Giacomo RIZZOLATTI (1992): Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study, Experimental brain research, 91 (1), pp. 176-180. <https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00230027>.

FRÉCHETTE, Guillaume (2011): Leibniz and Brentano on apperception, in H. Breger, J. Herbst y S. Erdner (eds.), Natur und subjekt. Vorträge, 1, Teil, Hanover, pp. 351-359.

GALLESE, Vittorio (2007): Before and below 'theory of mind': Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, pp. 659-669. <https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2002>.

GALLESE, Vittorio (2013): Corpo non mente. le neuroscienze cognitive e la genesi di soggettività ed intersoggettività, Educazione Sentimentale, 20, pp. 8-24. <https://doi.org/10.3280/eds2013-020002>.

GALLESE, Vittorio (2019): Embodied simulation. Its bearing on aesthetic experience and the dialogue between neuroscience and the humanities», Gestalt Theory, 41 (2), pp. 113-27. <https://doi.org/10.2478/gth-2019-0013>.

GALLESE, Vittorio, y Valentina CUCCIO (2015): The paradigmatic body. Embodied simulation, intersubjectivity, the bodily self, and language, in Thomas K. Metzinger, Jennifer M. Windt (eds.), Open MIND, MIND Group, Frankfurt am Main, pp. 1-22.

GALLESE, Vittorio, y Michele GUERRA (2015): Lo schermo empatico. Cinema e neuroscienze, Raffaello Cortina Editore, Milano.

GALLESE, Vittorio, y George LAKOFF (2005): The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22 (3-4), pp. 455-79. <https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000310>.

GALLESE, Vittorio, Paolo MIGONE y Morris N. EAGLE (2006): La simulazione incarnata: I neuroni specchio, le basi neurofisiologiche dell’intersoggettività ed alcune implicazioni per la psicoanalisi, Psicoterapia e scienze umane, XL (3), pp. 543-80. <http://digital.casalini.it/10.1400/65027>.

GARBARINI, Francesca, y Mauro ADENZATO (2004): At the root of embodied cognition: Cognitive science meets neurophysiology, Brain and Cognition, 56 (1), pp. 100-6. <https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.06.003>.

GARCÍA MÁRQUEZ, Gabriel (1967): Cien años de soledad, Sudamericana, Buenos Aires.

GENTRY, Gerad (2022): Pure synthesis and the principle of the synthetic unity of apperception», Kant-Studien, 113 (1), pp. 8-39. <https://doi.org/10.1515/kant-2022-2002>.

GIL GUERRERO, Herminia (2006): Lo fantástico como transgresión. Postulación fantástica en los relatos borgianos, in Nina Grabe, Sabine Lang and Klaus Meyer-Minnermann (eds.), La narración paradójica. Normas narrativas y el principio de la transgresión, Iberoamericana/Vervuert, Madrid, pp. 183-92. <https://doi.org/10.31819/9783964561626-011>.

GOLDMAN, Alvin I. (1989): Interpretation psychologized, Mind and Language, 4 (3), pp. 161-85. <https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1989.tb00249.x>.

GORDON, Robert M. (1986): Folk psychology as simulation, Mind and Language, 1 (2), pp. 158-71. <https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1986.tb00324.x>.

GRUBER, Nicole (2022): The implicit achievement motive in the writing style, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, pp. 1-22. <https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-022-09891-7>.

HEAL, Jane (1986): Replication and functionalism, in Jeremy Butterfield (ed.), Language, mind, and logic, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge , pp. 135-150.

JACKSON, Rosemary (1981): Fantasy. the literature of subversion, Routledge, London.

JAHN, Manfred (2011): Response to five questions on narrative theories and poetrics, in Peer F. Bundgaard, Henrik Skov Nielsen y Frederik Stjernfelt (eds.,) Five questions: Narrative theories and poetics, Automatic Press/VIP, New York, pp. 85-93. <http://www.uni-koeln.de/%7Eame02/jahn_2011.htm>.

JAHN, Manfred (2021): Narratology 2.3: A guide to the theory of narrative. <http://www.uni-koeln.de/~ame02/pppn.pdf>.

KIM, Alan (2016): Wilhelm Maximilian Wundt, in Edward N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, Stanford. <https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/wilhelm-wundt/>.

KIM, Alan (2015): Johann Friedrich Herbart, in Edward N. Zalta (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, Stanford. <https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/johann-herbart/>.

KLEINER, Johannes, y Erik HOEL (2021): Falsification and consciousness, Neuroscience of Consciousness, 1, niab001. <https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niab001>.

KLIEGL, Reinhold, Antje NUTHMANN y Ralf ENGBERT (2006): Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations, Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 135 (1), pp. 12-35. <https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.12>.

KWELDJU, Siusana (2015): Neurobiology research findings: How the brain works during reading, Pasaa, 50, pp. 125-42. <http://dx.doi.org/10.14456/pasaa.2015.5>.

LANG, Sabine (2006): Prolegómenos para una teoría de la narración paradójica, en Nina Grabe, Sabine Lang y Klaus Meyer-Minnermann (eds.), La narración paradójica. Normas narrativas y el principio de la transgresión, Iberoamericana/Vervuert, Madrid, pp. 21-47. <https://doi.org/10.31819/9783964561626-002>.

LEIBNIZ, Gottfried Wilhelm (1989): Philosophical papers and letters, trad. Leroy E. Loemker, Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht/Boston/London. <https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-1426-7>.

LUGNANI, Lucio (1983): Per una delimitazione del genere, in Remo Ceserani, Lucio Lugnani, Giulio Goggi, Carla Benedetti y Emanuela Scarano (eds.), La narrazione fantastica, Nistri-Lischi, Pisa, pp. 37-74.

MESÁROVÁ, Eva (2014): Discorso teorico-critico sul fantastico negli ultimi anni del Novecento in Italia, Romanica Olomucensia, 1, pp. 77-84. <https://doi.org/10.5507/ro.2014.007>.

NEGRI, Attà, Marco CASTIGLIONI, Cristina Liviana CALDIROLI y Arianna BARAZZETTI (2022): Language and Intelligence: A Relationship Supporting the Embodied Cognition Hypothesis, Journal of Intelligence, 10 (3), 42. <https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10030042>.

OLGOSO, Ángel (2007): Los demonios del lugar, Almuzara, Córdoba.

PACHECO SOARES, Marcelo (2019): O fantástico, uma vez mais, Études romanes de Brno, 40 (2), pp. 7-12. <https://doi.org/10.5817/erb2019-2-1>.

PATOINE, Pierre-Louis (2019a): Lectura inmersiva, lectura encarnada: Una aproximación neuroestética a la descripción del entorno en la obra de Antoine Volodine, Signa. Revista de la Asociación Española de Semiótica, 28, pp. 205-35. <https://doi.org/10.5944/signa.vol28.2019.25046>.

PATOINE, Pierre-Louis (2019b): Representation and immersion. The embodied meaning», Gestalt Theory, 41 (2), pp. 201-16. <https://doi.org/10.2478/gth-2019-0019>.

RAMACHANDRAN, Vilayanur (2022): Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind the great leap forward in human evolution, Edge Published online, June 29. https://www.edge.org/conversation/mirror-neurons-and-imitation-learning-as-the-driving-force-behind-the-great-leap-forward-in-human-evolution.

REMORINI, Paolo (2012): Fondamenti della poetica di Ángel Olgoso, Orillas. Rivista d'Ispanistica, 2, pp. 1-15. <http://orillas.cab.unipd.it/orillas/02_09remorini_rumbos/>.

REMORINI, Paolo (2022): Mixing the Frames. Horizontal and Vertical Paralepsis as a Metaleptic Tool for Activating the Fantastic Linkage within a Model of Cognitive and Hermeneutical Processes. The Case of Josef K. by Michele Mari, Zenodo .

REZA, Matthew (2015): Oltre il confine: La dilatazione dei paradigmi di realtà nei racconti fantastici di Dino Buzzati, Italianistica, 44 (3), pp. 127-39.

RIZZOLATTI, Giacomo, y Corrado SINIGAGLIA (2016): The mirror mechanism: a basic principle of brain function, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, pp. 757-765. <https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.135>.

ROAS, David (2011): Tras los límites de lo real. Una definición de lo fantástico, Páginas de Espuma, Madrid.

ROVELLI, Carlo (1996): Relational Quantum Mechanics, International Journal of Theoretical Physics, 35, pp. 1637-1678. <https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02302261>.

ROVELLI, Carlo (2014): La realtà non è come ci appare. La struttura elementare delle cose, Raffaello Cortina Editore, Milano.

ROVELLI, Carlo (2020): Helgoland, Adelphi, Milano.

SCHNEIDER, Ralf (2001): Toward a Cognitive Theory of Literary Character: The Dynamics of Mental-Model Construction, Style, 35 (4), pp. 607-639.

TODOROV, Tzvetan (1970): Introduction à la littérature fantastique, Editions du Seuil, París.

TORRENTE BALLESTER, Gonzalo (1972): La saga/fuga de J. B., Ediciones Destino, Barcelona.

ULICH, Robert (2022): Apperception, in Encyclopedia of philosophy. <https://www.encyclopedia.com/humanities/encyclopedias-almanacs-transcripts-and-maps/apperception>.

UMILTÀ, Maria Alessandra, Cristina BERCHIO, Mariateresa SESTITO, David FREEDBERG y Vittorio GALLESE (2012): Abstract art and cortical motor activation: an EEG study, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 311. <https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00311>.

WASSILIWIZKY, Eugen, y Winfried MENNINGHAUS (2021): Why and how should cognitive science care about aesthetics?, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25, pp. 437–449. <https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.03.008>.

WOJCIEHOWSKI, Hannah, y Vittorio GALLESE (2022): Embodied simulation and emotional engagement with fictional characters, in Patrick Colm Hogan, Bradley J. Irish y Lalita Pandit Hogan (eds.), The Routledge Companion to Literature and Emotion, Routledge, New York, pp. 61-73. <https://www.routledge.com/The-Routledge-Companion-to-Literature-and-Emotion/Hogan-Irish-Hogan/p/book/9780367409159>.

ZANGRANDI, Silvia (2017): Un tuffo nell’onomastica fantastica, ETS, Pisa.

ZENKINE, Serge (2021): Dostoïevski, Baxtin et le fantastique, Revue des Études Slaves, XCII (3-4), pp. 525-34. <https://doi.org/10.4000/res.4748>

Publicado

16-01-2023

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.